17
$\begingroup$

The following problem has bugged me for a while, ever since I noticed it. On the Visible Spectrum Wikipedia, the following is the visible spectrum:

colour spectrum

Now, in Photoshop, or really any colour picker, the hue slider looks something like this: photoshop hue selection

Or sometimes this:

colour wheel

I noticed that in both of these, the colour loops back to red. Why is this? I believe that this doesn't happen on the visible spectrum. The visible spectrum goes from a violet-ish colour to a maroon-ish colour, with a whole range in between. But where does the magenta colour from the hue slider fit in?

I take it that it is possible to have a purely yellow object, or a purely teal one, as it is on the colour spectrum, yet are magenta things and pink flowers inherently reflecting multiple wavelengths of light, from opposite ends of our particular viewing spectrum? All of this seems awfully odd to me, so I was hoping someone might be able to clear it up.

$\endgroup$
12
  • 4
    $\begingroup$ Google "color space" or see How does light combine to make new colours? $\endgroup$
    – mmesser314
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 3:44
  • 1
    $\begingroup$ A theory I have is that there's a sense in which it couldn't have been anything other than a wheel. A connected graph on 3 vertices (rgb) is either a triangle (same topology as a circle) or a line. Topologically there are only really two choices if we want color perception to be continuous. And I'd argue that the topology of a line doesn't really make sense if we can only perceive one color at a time (i.e. we don't perceive things as being blue and red). It seems natural that we would identify opposite ends of the spectrum to make sense of colors that are blue and red but not green. $\endgroup$ Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 11:40
  • 1
    $\begingroup$ In addition to the answers already given, keep in mind that the color wheel is not a physics tool, it’s an artistic tool. It helps the artistically-challenged such as myself to find colors that most people feel “look nice” together. I can get two complementary colors, three equidistant over the circle, or perhaps three close together, but reasonably equally spaced (aka the artist’s definition of “monochromatic” ;)). My wife likes those pictures better than when I make color decisions without this crutch ;) $\endgroup$
    – W_vH
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 19:33
  • 2
    $\begingroup$ "But where does the magenta colour from the hue slider fit in?" - nowhere. Magenta/pink does not have a corresponding frequency. It's a visual sensation you get when both the red cones and the blue cones are activated in your eyes. That's why it loops back - because of how your eyes work, and because your brain bridges the gap. $\endgroup$ Commented Aug 26, 2023 at 10:06
  • 1
    $\begingroup$ @RobbieGoodwin - The question is not about the convention or the helpfulness of the color wheel, the question is why are there no purples in the visible light spectrum and how come it's possible to create the loop in the first place. $\endgroup$ Commented Aug 27, 2023 at 1:51

3 Answers 3

24
$\begingroup$

The monochromatic spectrum and the hue wheel are different 1D paths through 3D color space

Since human color vision involves three types of cone cells, all the colors a human can see can be represented as a region of 3D space.

The most straightforward of these spaces is LMS space (long, medium, and short wavelength), where the coordinates of a point in space represent the stimulation of each type of cone cell. LMS space can be transformed with a linear transformation to the more convenient XYZ space. The axes of XYZ space were chosen to have useful properties, but for our purposes, it just produces nicer looking plots. Note that not all points in LMS space or XYZ space represent colors humans can see; for instance, a cone can't be stimulated by a negative amount.

The below "chromaticity diagram" is a plot of a 2D slice of XYZ color space in the plane $X + Y + Z = 1$.

chromaticity diagram

The colored region of the plot, label "visible region", contains points in the space representing colors humans can see, while the non-colored region contains points representing physically impossible cone stimulations. The top edge of the colored region corresponds to monochromatic colors. The circled vertices of the triangle labeled "sRGB region" represent the red, green, and blue primary colors used by an sRGB monitor, and the edges and interior of the triangle are linear combinations of the primaries. Note that only colors in the sRGB region can be reproduced by an sRGB monitor, so colors outside the region in the diagram are clamped to nearby sRGB colors.

Finally, the answer: the visible light spectrum is the non-closed path along the top edge of the colored region, and the hue wheel is some closed path through the colored region. The exact path of the hue wheel varies, but it is often chosen to be the perimeter of the triangle defined by the three sRGB primaries.

Notice that the bottom edge of this triangle approaches the bottom edge of the colored region, which does not represent monochromatic colors, but instead combinations of red and blue, giving magenta.

$\endgroup$
9
  • 5
    $\begingroup$ And there's a name for the line along the bottom of the space (which is nearly the same as the line along the bottom of the triangle, if your primaries are decent): the line of purples. That's what brings you back to red, and magenta lives approximately in the center of it. $\endgroup$
    – hobbs
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 18:04
  • $\begingroup$ Something I find helpful in understanding the chromacity diagram here: this diagram was constructed empirically. They asked people to distinguish between nearby pairs of colors and found that if they plotted the data in that particular way, the distance between any two colors corresponded to how different they were (how well people could distinguish them)\ $\endgroup$
    – Cort Ammon
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 18:22
  • 2
    $\begingroup$ @CortAmmon not really, the XYZ (CIE 1931) space doesn't encode perceptual distance. Its main objective is to make all the color matching functions nonnegative (which you can't achieve with any choice of "possible" RGB primaries). CIELUV is an actual attempt to make the color space perceptually uniform. $\endgroup$
    – Ruslan
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 18:24
  • 3
    $\begingroup$ @Vaelus "The circled vertices of the triangle represent the monochromatic red, green, and blue primary colors used by an sRGB monitor" — this is wrong, the triangle corresponds to CIE RGB, not sRGB. The latter represents a much smaller portion of the visible gamut. $\endgroup$
    – Ruslan
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 18:27
  • 3
    $\begingroup$ While chromacity diagrams are very useful to better understand color, remember that you are most likely looking at these pictures displyed on an RGB screen, which can't properly render the colors outside that little RGB gamut triangle. The same problem applies to the monochromatic color charts. You'll have to experience those colors AFK to really see the difference. $\endgroup$
    – jkej
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 22:45
10
$\begingroup$

The reason that color wheels are so common is that when certain colors are absorbed by materials, and thus not effectively reflected back towards a human's eyes, humans tend to perceive the material as being the "complementary color" on the opposite side of the color wheel to the color absorbed. This, as you rightly pointed out, has nothing at all to do with the actual electromagnetic frequencies of the light itself. Rather, it has to do with the peculiarities of how sensitive the human eye's distinct cell receptors are to different frequencies of electromagnetic spectrum. The three cell types, represented by the three distinct curves on this graph, have the color perception sensitivities shown.

Sensitivites of eye cells to electromagnetic frequencies taken from https://www.researchgate.net/figure/The-three-color-systems-in-normal-human-color-vision-2_fig4_323084928

The total obtained by adding up the various contributions of these curves looks like the following figure.

Total spectral sensitivity of the human eye taken from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Color_vision

It just so happens that when you absorb, e.g. red light, that the majority of the intensity remaining will tend to be of greener color than anticipated, making you perceive the complementary color green. Likewise, when green colors are absorbed, the remaining colors are mostly red, making you perceive the complementary color red. The color wheel just neatly organizes this otherwise highly unintuitive information for us.

color wheel image taken from https://www.hgtv.com/design/decorating/design-101/color-wheel-primer

Rather than display the raw electromagnetic spectral values of colors, which is far more useful to a physicist, color wheels show us the relationships between colors as humans actually perceive them, which is far more useful to an artist, for instance. Of course, the fact that these are continuous spectra and the fact that materials can absorb to different degrees over wide varieties of wavelengths can make the actual prediction of optical properties of materials quite difficult a priori. This is just a useful heuristic for simple examples.

$\endgroup$
9
  • 3
    $\begingroup$ Note also that hue typically consists of composite colours whereas any given point on the spectrum that physicists use is essentially monochromatic. $\endgroup$
    – Rodney
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 11:06
  • 1
    $\begingroup$ Thank you for saying more clearly what I was trying to express at the end there! $\endgroup$ Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 11:31
  • 2
    $\begingroup$ I think this does not answer the question. First, the sensation of colour comes from light directly, there's no need to explain it using objects, materials, or absorption: just look at a light source directly. Second, this talks about "complementary colours" without explaining first why there is a magenta, purple, etc. colour when there's no spectral magenta, purple, etc. $\endgroup$
    – Pablo H
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 14:02
  • 3
    $\begingroup$ @MattHanson Except, the colour wheel is more psychological / neurobiological than this "composite" explanation you give. Filter the red frequencies from a white colour source, and I wouldn't call the resulting colour green, but cyan. Filter the peak (green-ish) frequencies of sunlight, and you get a purplish colour (not red). This isn't why humans perceive red-green and yellow-blue as complementary pairs. $\endgroup$
    – wizzwizz4
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 14:53
  • 2
    $\begingroup$ @MattHanson, just to make it more fun: when mixing light (not paint) the color wheel changes. There’s a nice example in this page: diyphotography.net/avoid-colour-banding-using-coloured-gels. I have that open on my phone whenever I’m in the studio ;) $\endgroup$
    – W_vH
    Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 19:25
6
$\begingroup$

The reason for your confusion is that color words are used in two incompatible ways.

  1. Humans with standard trichromatic vision have three types of cones. The outputs of the three types of cone can be seen as coordinates in a three-dimensional space of colors. Any beam of light has a single color, which is a function of the cone response it produces if you shine it into someone's eye. Magenta light exists; otherwise we wouldn't see that color.

  2. Unfortunately, people also use color names like red and blue for single frequencies of light. They call a light beam whose Fourier spectrum has one narrow peak "monochromatic", and they say that a light beam with two peaks in the Fourier spectrum has two colors, even though you will see only one color when you look at it, and that color is not either of the two colors they assign to it.

This leads to endless confusion, including the idea that magenta is not a real color (just because there isn't any single frequency of light that can be given the name magenta in the second scheme).

In the rest of this answer I'll use color words only in the first sense.

Practically every colored object in nature, whether it's teal or magenta, reflects (scatters) a wide range of frequencies of light. The color of the scattered light is no less "pure" for all that.

Teal and magenta do differ in that teal can be produced by light with a single local maximum in its Fourier spectrum, while magenta needs two local maxima, or at least a local minimum in the middle of the visible range. That's quite different, though, from saying that teal is a single frequency and magenta is two frequencies. They aren't one frequency or two frequencies; they are colors.

We evolved color vision to see ripe fruit and saber-toothed tigers, not rainbows (probably). In terms of Fourier spectra, a rainbow is simple, but perceptually, it's a sort of random walk through the 3D color space. It could make it around the whole hue wheel, but happens not to—this is more or less an accident of how the cone pigments work. It could stop and reverse direction on the hue wheel, and I think it actually does do so at the long-wavelength end, but the colors are so dim there that it's hard to see.

Beyond that, see Vaelus's answer—but where it says "monochromatic", think "Fourier component".

$\endgroup$
1
  • 1
    $\begingroup$ I really like that you brought in the ideal examples of Fourier decompositions monochromatic light and contrasted that with colors that are composed of mixtures despite being perceptually pure. $\endgroup$ Commented Aug 25, 2023 at 19:25

Not the answer you're looking for? Browse other questions tagged or ask your own question.